Skip to content

Is There Anything Wrong With Incest? Emotion, Reason and Altruism in Moral Psychology

Meet Julie and Mark, two siblings who are vacationing together in France. One night after dinner and a few bottles of wine, they decide to have sex. Julie is on the pill and Mark uses a condom so there is virtually no chance that Julie will become pregnant. They enjoy it very much but decide to never tell anyone or do it again. In the end, having sex brought them together and they are closer than ever.

Did Julie and Mark do anything wrong?

If incest isn’t your thing, your gut-reaction is probably yes – what Julie and Mark did is wrong. But the point of Julie and Mark’s story, which was created by University of Virginia professor of social psychology Jonathan Haidt, is to illustrate how easy it is to feel that something is wrong and how difficult it is to justify why something is wrong. This is what happens when Haidt tells the Julie and Mark story to his undergrads. Some say that incest causes birth defects, or that Julie and Mark will cause pain and awkwardness to friends and family, but birth control and secrecy ensured that none of these problems will occur. Students who press the issue eventually run out of reasons and fall back on the notion of it  “just being wrong.” Haidt’s point is that “the emotional brain generates the verdict. It determines what is right and what is wrong… The rational brain, on the other hand, explains the verdict. It provides reason, but those reasons all come after the fact.”

So the question is: when it comes to our moral sentiments and deliberations, what system is in charge, the rational one or the emotional one?

The reason-emotion debate runs throughout the field of moral psychology. On one hand, cognitive science clearly shows that emotion is essential to our rationality, on the other hand, psychologists argue if reason really is the “slave of the passions,” as David Hume suggested. Haidt tends to take on the later position (and this is what the incest debate illustrates), but psychologists such as Paul Bloom and Steven Pinker believe that reason can persuade our emotions; this is why we have moral progress they argue.

Neuroscience is weighing in too. It demonstrates that we use different parts of the brain when we think deliberately versus when we go with our guts. As one author explains, “subjects who choose [rationally] rely on the regions of the brain known as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the posterior parietal cortex, which are known to be important for deliberative reasoning. On the other hand, people who decide [with their guts] rely more on regions of the limbic cortex, which are more closely tied to emotion.”

So which system sets the agenda, the intuitive one or the rational one? Should I go with my gut as Gladwell advertises? Or would that lead me into predictably irrational mistakes as Ariely warns? Should I listen to my unconscious as Gerd Gigerenzer and Timothy Wilson suggest? Or, as the Invisible Gorilla folks advise, should I take note of how intuitions deceive us? And finally, will we ever know if anything is objective wrong with incest?

Moral psychology is young, so are relevant neuroscience and evolutionary psychology studies, so I hesitant to draw any conclusions here. So what about more general moral feelings? Is it nature, nurture, or somewhere in between? Thanks to several recent studies we now have some answers.

One experiment, which I briefly mentioned a couple of months ago, comes from Paul Bloom, Kiley Hamlin and Karen Wynn. Bloom summarizes in the following article:

In one of our first studies of moral evaluation, we decided… to use… a three-dimensional display in which real geometrical objects, manipulated like puppets, acted out the helping/hindering situations: a yellow square would help the circle up the hill; a red triangle would push it down. After showing the babies the scene, the experimenter placed the helper and the hinderer on a tray and brought them to the child. In this instance, we opted to record… which character they reached for, on the theory that what a baby reaches for is a reliable indicator of what a baby wants. In the end, we found that 6- and 10-month-old infants overwhelmingly preferred the helpful individual to the hindering individual.

Does this mean that we are born with a moral code? No, but it does suggest that we have a sense of compassion and favor those who are altruistic from very early on.

Another experiment comes from Marco Schmidt and Jessica Sommerville. Schmidt and Sommerville showed 15 months year old babies two videos, one in which an experimenter distributes an equal share of crackers to two recipients and another in which the experimenter distributes an unequal share of crackers (she also did the same procedure with milk). Then, they measured how the babies looked at the crackers and milk while they were distributed. According to “violation of expectancy,” babies pay more attention to something when it surprises them. This is exactly what they found; babies spent more time looking when one recipient got more food than the other.

What does this suggest? According to the researchers, “the infants expected an equal and fair distribution of food, and they were surprised to see one person given more crackers or milk than the other.” This doesn’t mean that the babies felt something was morally wrong, but it does mean that they noticed something wasn’t equal or fair.

Schmidt and Sommerville followed up the experiment with another. In the second, they offered the babies two toys, a LEGO block and a LEGO doll. They labeled whichever toy the babies chose as their preferred toy. Then an experimenter asked the baby if he could have the preferred toy. They found that about one-third of the babies gave away their preferred toy, another third gave away the toy that wasn’t preferred, and the last third didn’t share at all. They also found that 92 percent of the babies who shared their preferred toy spent considerably more time looking when the food was unequally distributed; 86 percent of babies who shared their less-preferred toy were more surprised when there was an equal distribution of food. In other words, the altruistic sharers (those who gave the preferred dolls away) noticed more when the crackers and milk weren’t distributed equally while the selfish sharers (those who gave the less-perferred dolls away) showed the opposite.

Taken together, Bloom’s and Schmidt and Sommerville’s work encourages the fact that our moral instincts form early on. But these two studies are just a tiny sampling. It is still difficult to say with certainty if we are born with a moral instinct or not. It is also difficult to say what this moral instinct entails.

Back to incest.

To be sure, evolutionary psychology easily explains why we morally reject incest  – obviously, reproducing with our siblings would be counter productive – but there are many other topics such as why we act altruistically, why we show compassion towards strangers and why we give to charity that remain fairly mysterious. Fortunately, moral psychology is making great progress. It is an exciting new field and I look forward to more findings like the ones outlined here. In addition, I hope that one day in the near future psychologists will come to a consensus regarding the emotion-reason debate.

Schmidt, M., & Sommerville, J. (2011). Fairness Expectations and Altruistic Sharing in 15-Month-Old Human Infants PLoS ONE, 6 (10) DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023223

61 Comments Post a comment
  1. I tend to belive that the only innate moral reasoning we have is expressed when there is an injustice or when we perceive a situation to be unequitable. From an evolutionary perspective, this could serve to ensure cooperation during a hunt, sharing of the kill and delegation of tasks. My working theory is that we are all born as self-centered beings whose own selfishness serves to socially condition eachother. We witness injustice in seeing that baby horde all the toys and learn sympathetically that it is wrong. Of course, we need our parents to articulate this to us through tireless prompting to share and say please and thank you but the message is reinforced by the experiences we have eary in life in the actual and proverbial sandbox.

    October 10, 2011
    • sammcnerney #

      Part of me agrees, and Steven Pinker’s latest book “The Better Nature of Our Angels,” spends a whole chapter exploring what you are describing. Peter Singer and Robert Wright also share similar theories if you are really interested. But I also think we have (and I didn’t mention this in the post) an innate moral sense towards our kin. Across all cultures we act altruistically towards our family and friends – those in the immediate circle.

      October 10, 2011
      • Thank you for the leads. I’ll add those to the list. The kin factor is always fascinating. I partailly agree with your theory but I would change your statement that we have an “innate moral sense owards our kin” to an innate capacity of a moral sense to whomever we come to know as kin. It’s probably splitting hairs but I do think we tend to place too much value on blood when it comes to our kin.

        October 10, 2011
        • sammcnerney #

          Mmm. Interesting. Well, seems to me that babies know right away who Mom is. So I wouldn’t say “whomever we come to know as kin.” Maybe i’ll give you brothers and sister or grandparents. Just not sure about moms.

          October 10, 2011
  2. One doesn’t normally think about babies having an innate sense of justice. But, that instinct has to start somewhere. It just later that it goes off the rails for some people. It makes sense though that through cultural evolution a sense of justice and altruism would develop as a means of living in groups.

    October 10, 2011
    • sammcnerney #

      “It makes sense though that through cultural evolution a sense of justice and altruism would develop as a means of living in groups.”

      Yes – that is Robert Wright’s argument – he’s got a big book on it (never read it though).

      And I would say that babies don’t have an innate sense of justice, but that they are born with brains that are ready to gain a sense of justice. Like language, we aren’t born with language, but we are born with the capacity to learn a language (These are heavy hitting debates though, tons more to be said about this stuff)

      October 10, 2011
    • I should also say that perhaps “fairness” is a better term. Justice has too many legal connotations.

      October 10, 2011
      • Gottfried Leibniz is one of the greatest SOCRATICS beausce he challanges everyday “common sense” notions of causality and of life. Significantly, Kant teaches us in the Introduction to his “Prolegomena to Any Future Metaphysics” that “common sense” is a meaningless answer to the puzzling arguments of David Hume. Rather, one must approach such puzzles in terms of “justified true belief theory.” The perspectives and conclusions of Leibniz constantly challenges the taken-for-granted justified true beliefs which prevent one from thinking creatively. Leibniz and Schopenhauer have the most unique metaphysical systems, in my opinion. According to David Dilworth in “Philosophy in World Perspective” (1989), [The four-by-four matrix composed by Dilworth has four hermeneutical variables in the horizontal plane. They correspond to Aristotle’s Four Causes. In the vertical plane are the four “core” ancient Greek philosophical “profiles”: the school of Aristotle, Plato, Democritus, and the Sophists.] Schopenhauer has the most unique concatenation of four hermeneutical variables in the history of philosophy. Dilworth describes Leibniz as an “atomistic Platonist,” or a “Platonic Democritean.” Leibniz somehow finds the elusive “middle terms” (sensus de communis) between these contradictory philosophical approaches! He thereby Socratically leads such everyday folk as myself to search for the wonders found in […] +5

        December 19, 2012
  3. Anonymous #

    Does anyone else notice that the part describing the display of geometrical objects is completly wrong? There is no yellow square, the circle isn’t the one being helped up the hill, there is no red triangle and the triangle isn’t hindering any shape.

    April 6, 2012
  4. I am an engineer and a level heeadd guy and I don’t have much time for mumbo jumbo. My wife who I have been with for 15 years is an artist and has always been interested in esoteric subjects astrology, mythology, magic and psychotherapy. Over the past year she had started doing astrology charts and tarot readings for friends, word spread and now she gives several readings a week and makes about a3500 + from doing this, she could make more but turns down work as her main work is still her art. She has a 1st class BA and a Masters degree in fine art and she also works providing group art therapy as well as studying art therapy. I worry that she could be damaging her reputation by doing fortune telling on the side. Someone at work found out and make jokes about it but some of the women have asked about getting readings. I just don’t know how to take it as she is a highly intelligent and educated woman and yet she is perhaps wasting her time on this bizarre occupation. How would you feel… +3Was this answer helpful?

    December 20, 2012
  5. Gottfried Leibniz is one of the greatest SOCRATICS beuasce he challanges everyday “common sense” notions of causality and of life. Significantly, Kant teaches us in the Introduction to his “Prolegomena to Any Future Metaphysics” that “common sense” is a meaningless answer to the puzzling arguments of David Hume. Rather, one must approach such puzzles in terms of “justified true belief theory.” The perspectives and conclusions of Leibniz constantly challenges the taken-for-granted justified true beliefs which prevent one from thinking creatively. Leibniz and Schopenhauer have the most unique metaphysical systems, in my opinion. According to David Dilworth in “Philosophy in World Perspective” (1989), [The four-by-four matrix composed by Dilworth has four hermeneutical variables in the horizontal plane. They correspond to Aristotle’s Four Causes. In the vertical plane are the four “core” ancient Greek philosophical “profiles”: the school of Aristotle, Plato, Democritus, and the Sophists.] Schopenhauer has the most unique concatenation of four hermeneutical variables in the history of philosophy. Dilworth describes Leibniz as an “atomistic Platonist,” or a “Platonic Democritean.” Leibniz somehow finds the elusive “middle terms” (sensus de communis) between these contradictory philosophical approaches! He thereby Socratically leads such everyday folk as myself to search for the wonders found in […] Was this answer helpful?

    August 1, 2013
  6. Thanks for helping me to see things in a different light.

    August 30, 2013
  7. I’m glad that we have an awesome no bshlulit work staff now. Shawn, Jesse, Brian, Ron, Chris, Mike and Ryan with a TON of Chris’ awesome friends thrown in. IF is a lot better now. I’m not hating on the one’s who left but I’m more happy with the people here now!

    August 31, 2013
  8. These topics are so confusing but this helped me get the job done.

    October 21, 2013

Trackbacks & Pingbacks

  1. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Papers Gulf
  2. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - One Stop Essay
  3. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - blitzchartered
  4. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - My Blog
  5. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Assignment Excellence
  6. What does sentiment or emotion have to do with the example above? - Solved Blog
  7. Meet Julie and Mark, two siblings who are vacationing together in France. One night after dinner and a few bottles of wine, they decide to have sex. Julie is on the pill and Mark uses a condom so there is virtually no chance that Julie will become pregnan
  8. Week 13 Discussion *Don’t forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie – versedwriters
  9. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - writerscharter
  10. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - blitzarchive.com
  11. Did Julie and Mark do anything wrong? – MyDiscussionBoards
  12. What does David Hume say about virtue and emotion (sentiment) (p. 404)? - Course Hero
  13. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Keen Papers
  14. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Weblog.blitzarchive
  15. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Robust Papers
  16. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Homework Secure
  17. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Online Assignment
  18. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Academiaessay
  19. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - skilledessays
  20. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie
  21. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Blog.premiumessays
  22. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Blitz
  23. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - School Core
  24. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Kesi Essays
  25. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Home Work Crackers
  26. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie | Authority Essays
  27. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Academic Paper Gurus
  28. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie | Pundit WritersPundit Writers
  29. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie | Complex Essay Helpers
  30. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie | Academic Essays
  31. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie | College Assignments
  32. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Great Essay Helpers
  33. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie | Academic Essay Cove
  34. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie | My Blog
  35. Week 13 Discussion *Don’t forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie – Assignment Writer Bay
  36. Week 13 Discussion *Don’t forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie – Essay writers Cove
  37. Week 13 Discussion *Don’t forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie – Essay Major
  38. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie
  39. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - essayconsortium
  40. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Essay Workshop
  41. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Essay Home
  42. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Essay 4 Money
  43. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Essay Cove
  44. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Essay Hub
  45. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - HOMEWORK BREAK
  46. Week 13 Discussion *Don’t forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - Essay Soldier Questions & Answers
  47. Week 13 Discussion *Don't forget to reply to at least one classmate. Meet Julie - we.blitzchartered

Leave a Reply, Be Constructive, and Let's Debate